Educational Methods & Psychometrics (EMP) ISSN: 2943-873X

Educational Methods & Psychometrics (EMP)

ISSN: 2943-873X

Full Html


The Role of Fluid Intelligence in EFL Learners’ Mental Lexicon


Fatemeh Livdani Torghabeh, Mona Tabatabaee-Yazdi

Tabaran Institute of Higher Education, Mashhad, Iran

Tabaran Institute of Higher Education, Mashhad, Iran

Keywords: EFL learners, fluid intelligence, mental lexicon

Full Html

1 | Introduction

Intelligence is a multifaceted and complex cognitive ability encompassing various mental skills such as problem-solving, learning, reasoning, and adapting to new situations (Gardner, 2004). It involves the capacity to acquire, process, and apply information effectively to navigate challenges and make informed decisions. Intelligence is not limited to a singular dimension but includes a combination of logical, creative, and practical elements. It enables individuals to understand, learn from experience, and engage with the world in adaptive ways, contributing to their overall ability to succeed in various aspects of life.

According to Gardner (2004), intelligence is a multilayered cognitive ability including problem-solving. It is a composite of various functions, allowing individuals to find effective solutions to challenges. Besides, Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory was introduced as a framework classifying cognitive abilities into ten groups, including fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence. CHC theory made a distinction between fluid intelligence (gf) and crystallized intelligence (gc); with gf representing skills essential for advanced mental processes, critical thinking, and reasoning, while gc encompasses knowledge gained through formal education and life experiences (Baghaei & Tabatabaee-Yazdi, 2015; Cattell, 1943; Vieira et al., 2022). The importance of fluid intelligence in problem-solving and its role in predicting various life outcomes, including academic performance and achievements, has been highlighted by numerous scholars (e.g., Carpenter et al., 1990; Chuderski, 2022; Egeland et al., 2022).

Mental lexicon often likened to a mental dictionary (Elman, 2004; Kenett & Hills, 2022; Vitevitch, 2020) is also a dynamic system influencing language processing (Aitchison, 2012; Castro & Siew, 2020). It shows the interconnectedness of words, semantic relationships, and associations within the mental lexicon, along with the impact of orthographic and phonological features. Various studies on word associations, lexical access, and the organizational structure of the mental lexicon have indicated the context-dependent nature of word meanings and the role of mental lexicon in language processing (Aitchison, 2003; de Sousa & Gabriel, 2015; Elman, 2004; Farahian, 2011; Kenett & Hills, 2022; Issa et al., 2022; Jayantha et al., 2022; Vitevitch, 2020).

This study aims to delve into the concept of intelligence, emphasizing the CHC theory, focusing on fluid intelligence (gf) as a predictor of mental lexicon. It highlights intelligence as a composite of multiple functions rather than a singular entity (Anastasi, 1992; Lenat & Feigenbaum, 1991). Accordingly, the study aims to investigate the relationship between fluid intelligence and the mental lexicon of Iranian English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners. It also explores potential gender differences in both mental lexicon and fluid intelligence among the participants.

2 | Method

2.1 | Participants and Setting

The sample of the current study included 200 Iranian intermediate EFL students (116 females and 84 males) who were selected based on convenience sampling. Their age ranged from 17 to 25 years old (Mean age = 22.6 years, SD = 1.8). All the participants’ mother tongue was Persian.

2.2 | Instruments

2.2.1 | Baddeley’s 3-min Grammatical Reasoning Test

To assess participants’ fluid intelligence, the Persian Adaption of Baddeley’s 3-minute grammatical reasoning test developed and validated by Baghaei et al. (2017) was used. The test includes sixty-four true/false sentences that should be completed in 3 minutes. The reliability of the test was reported to be 0.91 (Baghaei et al., 2017).

2.2.2 | Lex30 Test

To measure the participants’ mental lexicon, Lex30 test developed and validated by Meara and Fitzpatrick (2000) was used. It is a word association task (WAT) in which test takers are given a list of stimulus words and asked to respond to these words with at least three related words or phrases that come to their mind. All of the words on the test were carefully selected from the most widely used Nation's first 1000 wordlist (1984) because the test was designed to be administered at a variety of proficiency levels. There were 30 stimulus words in the test, and participants had 30 seconds to respond to each one.

3 | Results

To examine the correlation between Iranian EFL learners' fluid intelligence and their mental lexicon, Spearman’s rho correlation was used due to the violation of parametric tests’ assumptions in the data. There was a moderate positive correlation between Iranian EFL learners' fluid intelligence and their mental lexicon with (rho = 0.33, p = 0. 00). Besides, the Mann-Whitney U test was used (Tables 1 and 2) to examine any significant differences in females’ and males’ Mental Lexicon and Fluid Intelligence.

As Table 1 shows, male learners have a higher Mean Rank score in their both Mental Lexicon (Males = 102.20, Females = 99.27) and Fluid Intelligence (Males = 108.97, Females = 94.37). To find whether the differences are significant, the Mann-Whitney U test (Table 2), the nonparametric alternative to the independent samples t-test, was conducted. The results indicated no significant difference between the two groups neither in their Mental Lexicon (Mann-Whitney U = 4729.00, Z = -0.354, p = 0.72) nor in their Fluid Intelligence (Mann-Whitney U = 4160.00, Z = -1.763, p = 0.07).

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics Concerning Gender

Participant

N

Mean

Mean Rank

Standard Deviation

Mental Lexicon

Female

116

45.49

99.27

0.87

Male

 84

45.92

102.20

1.04

Fluid Intelligence

Female

116

94.37

94.37

10948.50

Male

 84

108.97

108.97

9153.50

Table 2

Mann-Whitney Test

Mann-Whitney U

 4729.000

 4160.500

Wilcoxon W

 11515.000

 10946.500

Z

-0.354

-1.763

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

 0.723

 0.078

Note. a. Grouping Variable: Gender

4 | Conclusion

The study aimed to explore the relationship between Iranian EFL learners' fluid intelligence and their mental lexicon. The results showed that higher fluid intelligence correlates with a more extensive mental lexicon, with no significant gender impact. The findings highlight that vocabulary knowledge is crucial for English learning, influencing communicative competence. Therefore, educators should adapt teaching techniques considering learner diversity. While fluid intelligence contributes to the prediction of mental lexicon growth, other factors like motivation and exposure also play major roles. Higher fluid intelligence enhances efficient vocabulary processing and retrieval. Future research can focus on longitudinal studies, larger sample sizes, and different intelligence types, teaching approaches, and techniques which affect mental lexicon and fluid intelligence.

References

Aitchison, J. (2003). Words in mind; An introduction to the mental lexicon (3rd Ed.). Blackwell Publishing.

Aitchison, J. (2012). Words in the mind: An introduction to the mental lexicon (4th Ed.). John Wiley & Sons.

Anastasi, A. (1992). What counselors should know about the use and interpretation of psychological tests. Journal of

Counseling & Development, 70(5), 610615. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1992.tb01670.x

Baghaei, P., Khoshdel-Niyat, F., & Tabatabaee-Yazdi, M. (2017). The Persian adaptation of Baddeley’s 3-min grammatical

reasoning test. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30, 16. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0070-z

Baghaei, P., & Tabatabaee-Yazdi, M. (2015). The C-Test: An integrative measure of crystallized intelligence. Journal of

Intelligence, 3(2), 46−58. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence3020046

Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: a theoretical account of the

processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97(3), 404431.

https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404

Castro, N., & Siew, C. S. Q. (2020). Contributions of modern network science to the cognitive sciences: Revisiting research

spirals of representation and process. Proceedings. Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences, 476(2238),

20190825. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspa.2019.0825

Cattell, R. B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 40(3), 153−193.

https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0059973

Chuderski, A. (2022). Fluid intelligence emerges from representing relations. Journal of Intelligence, 10(3), 118.

https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030051

de Sousa, L. B., & Gabriel, R. (2015). Does the mental lexicon exist? Revista de Estudos da Linguagem, 23(2), 335−361.

http://dx.doi.org/10.17851/2237-2083.23.2.335-361

Egeland, J., Andreassen, T., & Lund, O. (2022). Factor structure of the new Scandinavian WISC?V version: Support for a

five?factor model. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 63(1), 1−7. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12780

Elman, J. L. (2004). An alternative view of the mental lexicon. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 301306.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.003

Farahian, M. (2011). Mental lexicon: its organization and representation. BRAIN. Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence

and Neuroscience, 2(4), 56−59. Retrieved from

https://lumenpublishing.com/journals/index.php/brain/article/view/1897

Gardner, H. (2004). A multiplicity of intelligences: In tribute to Professor Luigi Vigno. Retrieved from

http://www.howardgardner.com

Kenett, Y. N., & Hills, T. T. (2022). Editors’ introduction to networks of the mind: How can network science elucidate our

understanding of cognition? Topics in Cognitive Science, 14(1), 4553. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12598

Issa, S. H. M., Awadh, F. H. A., & Ahmed, H. R. (2022). The role of proficiency level in the speed of lexical activation.

Cogent Arts & Humanities, 9(1), 1999613. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311983.2021.1999613

Jayantha, K., Ariyawansa, R., & Kumara, U. (2022). Definition of definition. International Journal of Multidisciplinary

Studies, 9(1), 1−20. https://doi.org/10.4038/ijms.v9i1.154

Lenat, D. B., & Feigenbaum, E. A. (1991). On the thresholds of knowledge. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 1−3.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(91)90055-O

Meara, P., & Fitzpatrick, T. (2000). Lex30: An improved method of assessing productive vocabulary in an L2. System,

28(1), 1930. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0346-251X(99)00058-5

Nation, I. S. P. (1984). Vocabulary lists. English Language Institute Occasional Publication No.12, Victoria University of

Wellington.

Vieira, B. H., Pamplona, G. S. P., Fachinello, K., Silva, A. K., Foss, M. P., & Salmon, C. E. G. (2022). On the prediction of

human intelligence from neuroimaging: A systematic review of methods and reporting. Intelligence, 93, 101654.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2022.101654

Vitevitch, M. S. (2020). Network science in cognitive psychology. Routledge.

PUBLISHED

07-02-2024

ISSUE

Vol.2,2024

SECTION

Rapid Communication